Other angles on �Intelligence�
Beliefs about intelligence (mindsets)
Increasingly, reading about intelligence in an educational context will lead you to the work of Carol Dweck, under the heading of "self-theories" or "mindsets".
Her research is not so much about intelligence itself, as people's beliefs about it--their own in particular. She focuses on two possible (usually implicit) theories or assumptions about the characteristics of intelligence; they are implicit in that people (particularly children, to whom she pays most attention) rarely talk about them directly, but they betray them in how they discuss challenges, problems, difficulties, opportunities, successes and failures.
She gets at these mindsets with a simple self-test. Which of the following do you most agree with?
1. Your intelligence is something very basic about you that you can't change very much.
2. You can learn new things, but you can't really change how intelligent you are.
3. No matter how much intelligence you have, you can always change it quite a bit.
4. You can always substantially change how intelligent you are.
(from Dweck, 2006: 12)
To be fair, this may appear mind-bogglingly banal, but it is the simple bottom line of two decades or more of research: it is taken from Dweck's more recent book for a popular readership--you'll find it under "self-help" in the bookshop, rather than respectable psychology, as the title suggests: Mindset; how you can fulfil your potential London; Robinson, 2012.
Although the answers are arranged in a rough continuum, the first two represent what is currently referred to as a "fixed" mindset. (The labels have changed over the years, but I'll use the current versions.) The second two represent a "growth" mindset.
-
A fixed mindset assumes that you are born with a certain amount of intelligence, and it remains static.
-
A growth mindset assumes that your intelligence can be developed: you are not confined to what you were born with.
A little elaboration;
-
This is not just a matter of intelligence�you can make the same distinction in relation to musical ability, sporting prowess, sociability, and other qualities.
-
The history of the idea of intelligence has been riven with nature/nurture debates�which come down to this divide.
-
It doesn't matter, for Dweck's purposes, which of the mindsets is "true" (and of course their expression is so crude that neither of them is accurate); she is interested in the difference it makes to a person to believe one rather than another.
-
She takes pains to claim that one is not better than the other. But that is a little disingenuous; she treats the fixed position as without merit and destructive.
-
When things are going well, she argues, which mindset you have doesn't matter much, and that is supported by her research evidence. It is when the going gets tough that the growth mindset wins hands down:
Fixed |
Growth |
...leads to a desire to look smart and therefore a tendency to... | ...leads to a desire to learn and therefore a tendency to... |
...avoid challenges | ...embrace challenges |
...get defensive or give up easily | ...persist in the face of setbacks |
...see effort as fruitless or worse | ...see effort as the path to mastery |
...ignore useful negative feedback | ...learn from criticism |
...feel threatened by the success of others | ...find lessons and inspiration in the success of others |
As a result may plateau early and achieve less than full potential |
As a result, reach ever-higher levels of achievement |
(based on graphic in Dweck, 2006:245)
-
The tinted row of the table is the critical step in the argument, and it has been the focus of most of the empirical research reported in Dweck (2000).
-
The basic take-home message is that student feedback ("student" here refers mainly to children) needs to focus on the amount of effort, concrete achievement and improvement the learner has demonstrated, in order to promote a growth mindset. To attribute success (or of course failure) to apparent qualities of the individual� "You're really bright!" or "That's stupid"�is to encourage a fixed mindset. This is actually harder than it sounds.
Nevertheless�the message is useful. Petty (2006: 45-49; and see briefly here) links Dweck's work with other theories of motivation and explores the practicalities of using it in class.
Here is Dweck's site based on the Mindset book.
Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence
The distinction between "fluid" (Gf) and "crystallized" (Gc) intelligence is probably mainly of interest to theorists, but it does also have some implications for teaching. The "G" in the abbreviated labels refers to Spearman's g factor, so this is not yet another attempt to deconstruct the possible components of intelligence. Instead, it draws attention to the interaction of what (very broadly and roughly�I warned you!) we might call "native wit" and "experience" in the expression of intelligence in an individual. However, the theory does not suggest that fluid intelligence becomes "crystallized" with age; indeed, more recent work in the field suggests that the abilities associated with each type may be associated with different parts of the brain. The idea is particularly associated with Raymond Cattell and John L Horn, in a series of papers principally produced from the early 'sixties to the late 'seventies.
Fluid intelligence: Horn and Cattell (1967) defined it as "�the ability to perceive relationships independent of previous specific practice or instruction concerning those relationships." That is one of those definitions of limited use as we saw here. What appears to be more important is that it works through abstractions; it is through abstraction that we perceive the essences of objects and concepts and thus can compare them.
Crystallized intelligence, on the other hand draws on the stock of knowledge deposited in memory through experience.
Both work together, and in many practical situations, answers to problems can be arrived at with different mixes of the two. The research suggests that while both forms increase through childhood and adolescence, fluid intelligence peaks in adolescence and begins to decline from early middle age. Crystallized intelligence continues to grow as long as experience is accumulated and learned from.
- It is often suggested that mathematicians and physicists (both working in complex and abstract field) do their best work before the age of 40 or even 30, although it is difficult to find decent research on this. Scholars in other areas, particularly the humanities, may however continue to grow in stature until they drop�their work draws much more on the sedimented knowledge of a lifetime.
- There are parallels, too, with Bruner's "Cone of Experience"; fluid intelligence is required to engage with the "symbolic" abstract levels, and crystallized intelligence draws on that "enacted" history to work at that and the "iconic" levels.
- But most obviously there are implications for teaching adults, and particularly older adults. They (OK�we) may not be so nimble with the concepts and the arguments, but what we already know (or think we do) may well more than make up for that. That is both an enormous resource for the teacher which we ignore at our peril, and a great challenge, because we may have to confront established "wisdom" (a.k.a. "common sense") to make progress:
- See Resistance to Learning.
- and Watts (2012) and there are parallels with Kahneman (2011).
References
- Cattell R B (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. New York: Elsevier Science Pub. Co.
- Horn J L & Cattell R B (1967) "Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence." Acta Psychologica, 26, 107-129.
up-dated 16 Oct 14
Atherton J S (2013) Learning and Teaching; [On-line: UK] retrieved from
Original
material by James Atherton: last up-dated overall 10 February 2013
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 Unported License.
Search Learningandteaching.info and associated sites:
Save this on Delicious Tweet Click here to send to a friend
This site is independent and self-funded, although the contribution of the Higher Education Academy to its development via the award of a National Teaching Fellowship, in 2004 has been greatly appreciated. The site does not accept advertising or sponsorship (apart from what I am lumbered with on the reports from the site Search facility above), and invitations/proposals/demands will be ignored, as will SEO spam. I am of course not responsible for the content of any external links; any endorsement is on the basis only of my quixotic judgement. Suggestions for new pages and corrections of errors or reasonable disagreements are of course always welcome. I am not on FaceBook or LinkedIn.